1. Introduction
Episodic memory is the ability to store information about events that have transpired in the surrounding environment [
1,
2]. By enabling animals to make memory-based predictions, avoid potential threats and find the most efficient ways to obtain vital resources, this unique cognitive faculty supports adaptability to the ever-changing environmental conditions [
2]. Episodic memories requires the binding of information about the spatial/environmental context (“where”), the objects/landmarks encountered within the environment (“what”), the temporal order of appearance of the stimuli within the environment (“when”) and in most occasions, the presence of familiar and/or unknown conspecifics (“who”) [
1,
2]. The formation of associations between the different aspects of an event is highly-dependent on the hippocampus, specifically the dorsal section of the structure [
1,
2,
3,
4]. Newly formed memories are labile at first and their long-term storage requires the synthesis of specific plasticity-related proteins (PRPs) [
5]. The protein-dependent process that governs the transition of a memory from its short-term form into its stable long-term one is collectively known as cellular (or initial) memory consolidation [
5]. Importantly, hippocampal glutamatergic transmission, while indispensable for memory formation, could not alone trigger the expression of PRPs that characterizes initial memory consolidation. The catecholaminergic neurotransmitters, noradrenaline (NA) and dopamine (DA), as well as the glucocorticoid hormone cortisol (corticosterone in laboratory rodents) are essential for the long-term storage of memories [
6,
7,
8]. Emotional salience and environmental novelty are two of the main conditions capable of evoking the physiological state required for the prioritization of information in the hippocampus [
9,
10,
11]. During emotionally arousing and/or distinctly novel situations the pontine nucleus locus coeruleus rearranges the firing patterns of its neurons to increase the secretion of NA and DA in dorsal hippocampus [
12,
13,
14]. Thus, events lacking emotional relevance or other salient characteristics (like novelty) may not activate catecholaminergic release in the dorsal hippocampus to a degree that will allow their long-term stabilization and availability for later retrieval [
11].
Nevertheless, neutral stimuli encountered close to a salient event may also be significant since they could be used as predictors for particular aversive or rewarding outcomes in the future [
11]. The behavioral equivalent of the “Synaptic tagging and capture hypothesis” [
15], the “Behavioral tagging hypothesis”, is a learning and memory model that offers reliable evidence of how emotional or distinctly novel experiences could modulate the consolidation of closely occurring low-arousing or neutral events [
16]. According to this hypothesis, the encoding of a weak event (memory about neutral or low-emotional behavioral tasks) closely before or after the encoding of a second, strong event (emotionally arousing behavioral task), will induce structural and functional modifications in sets of synapses belonging to the cellular assembly that constitutes the trace of the memory (behavioral tag) [
11,
16,
17]. Subsequently, the elevated levels of catecholamines (or other neuromodulators like corticosterone) triggered by the strong event would activate the expression of PRPs needed to stabilize the induced alterations in the synapses and to consolidate the memory for the weak event. So far, the validity of the Behavioral tagging hypothesis has been confirmed by a plethora of studies combining weak protocols of various behavioral tasks (e.g. contextual fear conditioning, inhibitory avoidance, spatial object recognition, novel object recognition) with novel environment exposure (a session of open field (OF) exploration) [
18,
19,
20,
21]. Pairing exposure to OF with weak inhibitory avoidance protocol (wIA), Moncada and colleagues (2011) [
20] demonstrated the importance of NA for the stabilization of the behavioral tag after memory encoding. By infusing beta adrenoreceptor antagonist (propranolol) in the dentate gyrus (DG), the researchers were able to disrupt the beneficial effect of OF on the consolidation of the memory for the wIA task. Moreover, the same study was able to establish that blocking the catecholaminergic transmission did not interfere with the formation of short-term IA memory, thus highlighting the role of NA in PRPs provision. A different set of studies established acute stress as another emotional state capable of modulating the consolidation of otherwise forgetful experiences [
22,
23]. The study by da Cunha et al. (2022) [
23] provided evidence that mild stressful experiences like placing Wistar rats on an elevated platform (EP) one hour before learning is sufficient to induce a long-term memory for wIA. The observed effects were dependent on the activation of the glucocorticoid receptors (GRs), since the infusion of the GRs antagonist mifepristone in the CA1 field of the dorsal hippocampus abolished the storage of the memory for wIA. Another significant finding reported by the research team was that when EP exposure was combined with strong IA (sIA), the formation of a long-term memory for the behavioral task was impaired. This raises the possibility that intensity of the emotional states established by two events determines the dynamics of their interaction on the molecular level - two temporarily proximal salient events may compete for PRPs, leading to insufficient consolidation and fragmented memory.
Beside the hippocampus, emotionally charged or stressful events activate the amygdala - a heterogenous group of interconnected nuclei located in the temporal lobe and involved in the regulation of emotional behaviors [
24]. The amygdala is necessary for the encoding emotional valence, emotional learning and regulation of the stress responses [
24,
25,
26,
27]. Substantial evidence has been provided that during emotional events one particular subregion of the structure - the basolateral complex of the amygdala (BLA) - incorporates the arousal signals originating from inside the brain (greater NA release from the LC) and from the peripheral hormone systems (heighten corticosterone level) to foster a positive modulatory effect on the processes required for the initial consolidation of memories [
9,
28,
29,
30,
31,
32]. Therefore, the BLA could also influence the prioritization of information in the hippocampus [
33]. Since the BLA does not project directly to the dorsal hippocampus, the effect it exhibits on memory processes is reliant on its known anatomical connections with the entorhinal cortex, a cortical area that functions as a gateway and provides the hippocampus with sensory information originating from other specialized circuits [
34,
35]. Most studies that have established the modulatory role of the BLA on memory consolidation combined behavioral training of the experimental animals with pharmacological manipulations, mainly direct post-encoding infusions of different pharmacological agents [
9]. However, evidence has also been provided by experiments using electrophysiological and optogenetic stimulation of the BLA, either during training or immediately after [
36,
37,
38]. Importantly, post-training stimulation of the BLA affects the consolidation of various behavioral tasks regardless of the emotional valence of the training [
38,
39,
40,
41,
42,
43,
44]. These findings allow for comparison with the protocols used by the behavioral tagging studies, since the stimulation of the BLA may mimic the effects on memory consolidation produced by a strong modulatory event. On the contrary, it has been reported that the memory modulation capacities of BLA stimulation depend on the levels of emotional arousal induced by the learning event [
39,
42]. For example, Roozendaal et al. (2006) [
39] provided evidence that system injection of corticosterone immediately after the training trial of object recognition task (ORT) improved the performance of the animals during the test session 24h later. This effect was not observed in the animals that were substantially habituated to the training context before the beginning of the experiments, showing that habituation may reduce novelty-induced emotional arousal in the brain. Moreover, the same study reveals that infusion of propranolol in the BLA immediately after training blocks the corticosterone enhancement of memory consolidation, which demonstrates the role of BLA for the integration of arousal signals and the need for NA activation of the structure for the emotional modulation of memory formation. In a similar study, McReynolds et al. (2010) [
40] have provided further evidence about the interaction between corticosterone and NA in the BLA. They observed that a systemic injection of corticosterone immediately after an IA training session increased the levels of NA in the BLA after 15 minutes, which was estimated through in vivo microdialysis. Furthermore, the administration of corticosterone elevated the synaptic levels of the activity-regulated cytoskeletal-associated protein (Arc) and enhanced memory consolidation for the IA task. However, when propranolol was infused in the BLA immediately after IA training and corticosterone administration, both the effect on memory and Arc expression were attenuated. Arc is an immediate early gene, the product of which is highly important for the stabilization of the synaptic modifications inducted by learning [
45,
46,
47]. Several other publications have demonstrated that intra-BLA administration of the beta-adrenergic receptor agonist clenbuterol enhances memories for different tasks and increases the expression of Arc in the dorsal hippocampus [
41,
42,
44]. In a series of experiments, McReynolds and colleagues (2014) [
42] revealed that post-encoding infusion of clenbuterol in the BLA of rats had a positive effect on the consolidation of memories for both IA and ORT tasks. This was accompanied by an increase in the synaptic expression of Arc. Remarkably, the effects of clenbuterol on memory performance and Arc expression in the ORT experiment were noticeable only in the animals that were not subjected to habituation prior to the training. This once again illustrates that a certain degree of arousal must be induced during training in order for BLA to exhibit its modulatory function. Experiments examining BLA-induced augmentation of memory consolidation in the hippocampus like the ones described here have led to the proposal of the “Emotional tagging hypothesis” [
29]. This hypothetical account is concerned with the role of the various aspects of an emotional event - intensity, controllability, duration - in facilitating of the interaction between the BLA on one side and memory-related plasticity in the hippocampus on the other. Despite the great number of studies examining the emotional modulation of memory consolidation, a thorough behavioral protocol to study interactions mentioned above, like those implemented by the behavioral tagging studies, is still lacking.
In light of the research reviewed above, our present experimental work intends to establish a novel behavioral protocol that could provide further evidence for the role of BLA in memory modulation by taking advantage of the natural changes in homeostatic conditions induced by an emotionally charged event. That is to say, we aimed to demonstrate that endogenous activation of the BLA by a stimulus with innate emotional valence promotes the stabilization of the behavioral tag set by a previous weak event. To do this, we decided to couple OLT with predatory odor exposure (POE) in a novel cage two minutes after the training trial. The presence of predatory cues has been proven to evoke neuronal responses in the BLA [
48]. For the purpose of our experiments we used urine from the predatory species Canis latrans (coyote). Urine from predatory species is a highly aversive stimulus for rodents as it contains kairomones - natural chemical substances that serve as ecological signals informing the animal of the presence of potential predatory threat [
49,
50]. Previous studies have used coyote urine as unconditioned stimuli in hippocampus-dependent contextual fear conditioning paradigm [
51,
52]. Recent publication has also provided a detailed analysis of the repertoire of defensive behaviors of rats exposed to coyote urine [
53]. Considering all these factors, we hypothesized that POE may be a potent modulatory event capable of inducing strong effect on the memory consolidation for a weaker behavioral task. Moreover, we predicted that memory enhancement prompt by POE may be dependent on NA transmission in BLA. To test these ideas, our group conducted two separate experiments. In Experiment #1, rats were assigned to three groups depending on the behavioral procedures and pharmacological treatment. Animals from the control group received a bilateral infusion of the vehicle in the BLA upon the compilation of the sample phase of the OLT. The other two groups were subjected to OLT training and exposed to coyote urine, followed by infusion of either vehicle or propranolol in the BLA. Rats from all the groups were sacrificed 90 minutes following pharmacological treatment and both of their hippocampi were extracted to examine the outcomes of BLA manipulation on experience-dependent plasticity.
Experiment #2 followed the same protocol, but to establish possible retroactive enhancement of memory induced by POE, the animals from the three groups were submitted to a test 24 hours later. As in the first experiment, animals were sacrificed 90 minutes after the test and both hippocampi were collected to assess the retrieval-related changes. The selected tissue samples were used to assess the levels of the phosphorylated form of cAMP response element-binding protein (pCREB) and the expression of Arc through ELISA and western blotting (WB), respectively. We selected these molecular markers based on existing evidence for their experience-dependent activation and role in different memory processes [
45,
54,
55,
56].
3. Discussion
The experiments presented here were designed to demonstrate that NA activation of the BLA during emotionally arousing situations is critical for any retrograde enhancement effect on memory consolidation. As our protocol relied on exposing animals to a stimulus with an innate negative valence to induce emotional arousal rather than a pharmacological intervention, we aimed to provide direct evidence for the emotional aspects of the behavioral tagging. Our study is conceptually close to those by da Cunha and colleagues (2019) [
22] and da Cunha and collaborators (2021) [
23]. Specifically, the design of the 2019 study [
22] paired weak OLT (4 minutes sample session) with EP session 1 hour after the initial training and discovered that the induced state of acute stress was able to save the memory for OLT from forgetting. The observed effects were dependent on elevated levels of corticosterone in the dorsal hippocampus, since infusion of antagonists for both mineralocorticoid (MRs) and glucocorticoid receptors (GRs) in the CA1 field disrupted the effects induced by the stressful experience. Furthermore, they showed that when EP was coupled with the strong version of OLT (10 minutes long training trial), the formation of a memory for the task was impaired, highlighting once again the observation that two temporarily proximal strong events may compete for PRPs. Although this was not the focus of their experiment, the results obtained by the researchers regarding the contribution of hippocampal MRs and GRs to memory enhancement could be interpreted from the perspective of a BLA-dependent process. As mentioned above, both the studies by Roozendaal et al. (2006) [
39] and McReynolds at al. (2010) [
40] have the demonstrated that the beneficial effect of a systemic corticosterone injection on memory could be prevented by infusion of propranolol in the BLA. These observations further motivated us to investigate how endogenous NA release in BLA during emotionally arousing situations influences the dynamics of memory formation in the hippocampus.
The main finding of our study is that the animals that were exposed to the predator odor and infused with vehicle immediately immediately after the end of the OLT sample session showed a significant upregulation in their pCREB levels and a trend toward increased Arc expression in the hippocampus compared to those infused with propranolol. Most importantly, this effect was mediated by the BLA. This is evident by the results obtained from our first experiment, where the animals that were infused with vehicle immediately after completion of the sample session of the OLT and the exposure to the predatory odor showed notably higher availability of both markers compared to those infused with propranolol. Furthermore, the substantial elevation in pCREB levels and the tendency for higher Arc expression in the rats from the POE/veh group compared to the control group, highlights the influence of the emotional event on hippocampal plasticity. A previous study by Kabitzke, Silva and Wiedenmayer (2011) [
57] reported that using cat odor as an unconditioned stimulus in a contextual fear conditioning paradigm significantly increased pCREB levels in both dorsal and ventral hippocampus of juvenile rats. However, when an intraperitoneal injection of propranolol was administered after the exposure to the predatory cue, this experience-dependent effect was not observed and the animals showed no conditioned response when they were re-exposed to the training context 24 hours later, suggesting that the antagonist may have blocked the activation of CREB by NA and hindered the cellular mechanism of memory consolidation. Finally, post-training systemic injection of the non-selective beta-adrenergic blocker nadolol, which cannot cross the blood-brain barrier, had no effects on pCREB levels or the studied behavioral responses, suggesting that the effect on memory consolidation was facilitated by the endogenous release of NA in the brain. Our findings regarding pCREB extend the evidence described above, revealing that the effects of predatory cues on hippocampus-dependent memory and memory-induced behavioral responses are also influenced by the activation of the noradrenergic neurotransmission in the BLA. Importantly, in the experiment of Kabitzke, Silva and Wiedenmayer (2011) [
57], the hippocampal tissue was collected 60 minutes after odor exposure. In our study, both hippocampi were extracted 90 minutes after the completion of all experimental procedures. This time point was selected because we wanted to establish the possible existence of a prolonged window for CREB activation. Since CREB is one of the main transcription factors involved in memory consolidation [
55,
56], a prolonged activation of CREB can be interpreted as an indication for increased transcriptional activity of the genes responsible for PRPs. With regard to Arc, our results showed a tendency for increased expression of the protein in the animals from the POE/veh group. This further emphasizes the role of BLA in modulating memory-related plasticity in the hippocampus during emotionally charged events. The studies that have provided evidence for BLA-modulated Arc expression have used synaptoneurosome preparation to determine synaptic levels of the protein in the dorsal hippocampus [
40,
42,
44]. In addition, these studies have collected the samples at different timepoints and have shown that the peak concentration may be approximately 1 hour after training [
40,
44]. In our experiment we homogenized the entire hippocampus. This, together with the time point of tissue collection, may have limited our ability to obtain more detailed results. Nevertheless, we still were able to demonstrate the presence of an emotional arousal-dependent effect on Arc expression regulated by BLA.
Unfortunately, the second experiment was unable to provide firm evidence for a POE-induced modulatory influence on memory consolidation. This is evident by the lack of pronounced effect in the performance of the POE/veh group during the test session of the memory task. In line with the collected behavioral data, Arc expression in the test groups also showed no significant recognition-induced differences. Regarding the pCREB levels of the test groups, the results confirm the notion established by Moncada and Viola (2006) [
58] that a familiar spatial environment can reduce the phosphorylation of the protein. As this is a pilot study, the behavioral protocol we used may have had some drawbacks that must be carefully evaluated and eliminated in the future. These drawbacks may partially explain the lack of behavioral effect observed in
Experiment #2. Considering the behavioral procedures, animals in both experiments were habituated to the experimental context for 10 minutes, one day before the sample session of OLT and POE. This was done in order to limit the presence of possible anxiety-related and neophobic behaviors during the training. However, the studies by Roozendaal et al. (2006) [
39] and McReynolds et al. [
42] reported that habituation may neutralize the influence of post-encoding BLA activation on memory consolidation. In their paper, McReynolds et al. (2014) [
42] suggest that there may be a requirement for parallel arousal-induced activation of both the BLA and the efferent memory-related structures for the modulatory effect to occur and promote greater synaptic expression of Arc. Thus, by familiarizing the animals with the experimental context, the habituation session of the current experiments may have limited the modulatory influences of the BLA by reducing novelty-induced activation of the dorsal hippocampus. Notably, in a study by Ballarini and colleagues (2009) [
18] - that coupled weak spatial object recognition task with OF - animals were subjected to two habituation sessions, but the presence of an enhancing effect on memory consolidation was still observed. This suggests that the modulatory events characterized either by environmental novelty or the presence of emotionally arousing stimuli may promote memory consolidation through the activation of different neural circuits. Confirmation or rejection of this notion requires more behavioral tagging studies that include emotionally valenced stimuli in their modulatory events. Another factor that may have influenced the lack of noticeable modulatory effect on memory consolidation in our second experiment is the temporal window between the weak and the modulatory events. The rats underwent POE only two minutes after the end of the OLT sample session. This interval was chosen, because we wanted to design a more naturalistic behavioral paradigm. In natural environmental conditions emotional stimuli may appear surprisingly and influence the consolidation of the memories for the most proximal neutral stimuli or contextual features that could be used to optimize future memory-based predictions. Most behavioral tagging experiments that reported positive modulation on memory consolidation have exposed the animals to the strong event at different time points - ranging from 15 minutes to 1 hour before or after training [
18,
20,
22]. Importantly, da Cunha and colleagues (2022) [
23] found that acute stress (EP) experienced immediately after wIA did not impair the formation of memory for the task and facilitated its long-term stabilization. However, given the different nature of the emotionally arousing events selected in ours and theirs (POE vs. EP), currently we cannot rule out the possibility that the animals in our experiment may have formed a strong association between the two events that could have influenced their performance during the test session of OLT. When re-exposed to the experimental context the following day, animals may have expected to encounter the aversive stimuli once again, which may explain why we did not observe the memory-related behaviors we expected. In a recent review by Sazma, Shields and Yonelinas (2019) [
59], which examined the effects of post-encoding stress in animal and human studies, the authors have expressed similar concerns about the possible negative effect of acute stress on subsequent memory retrieval. Considering the fact that the POE was only performed for 4 minutes, we doubt that the intensity of our modulatory event could have negatively impacted the consolidation of the memory for OLT. Detailed studies on how different physiological parameters of the emotional response to different predatory odors might influence the cellular and molecular basis of memory processes are still lacking. Nevertheless, a recent report by Migliaro and colleagues (2019) [
60] provides some insights into the observed behavioral changes depending on the duration of the exposure. Using various tests to assess anxiety-like and depressive-like behavior 24 hours after exposure of Wistar rats to bobcat urine, they found that animals exposed for 10 or 20 minutes showed clear change in their behavioral profile, while those exposed to the predatory cue for 3 minutes showed no profound alterations. Taking all these different possibilities into account, we must emphasize that the results of our first experiment on pCREB and Arc expression directly argues against the likelihood of our modulatory event being too mild to produce experience-dependent neural activity in the hippocampus.
Author Contributions
Conceptualization, B.P. and J.T.; methodology, B.P,P.I.,D.K., L.K.,P.R.J,T.; formal analysis, B.P.,P.I.,J.T.; investigation, B.P.,P.I.,D.K.; resources, D.A.; data curation, B.P.; writing—original draft preparation, B.P.,J.T; writing—review and editing, B.P.,J.T.; project administration, D.A.; funding acquisition, D.A. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.